Building Cognitive Reserve using The Brain game
How Smart Memory Habits Can Protect Your Brain
The Mildot Brain Game eLearning Solution Applies These Techniques
.
Cognitive Reserve
The brain’s hidden strength. It’s what helps us stay sharp as we age or face life’s mental challenges. Simply put, a strong reserve means your brain can adapt and cope when things get tough (Stern, 2009).
Good news, you can grow your Cognitive Reserve through simple, science-backed habits. Memory systems play a big part. Your brain stores memories in two ways, declarative (facts and events) and procedural (skills and routines). When you train both, you build mental shortcuts that stay strong for life (Cabeza, Nyberg & Park, 2018).
Cognitive Reserve is not directly measurable like blood pressure or heart rate. It’s an idea based on scientific evidence showing that some people cope better with brain ageing or damage than others, even when their brains show similar physical changes (Stern, 2009).
Researchers measure it indirectly by looking at things like education level, lifelong learning, mentally stimulating activities, and performance on cognitive tests. These clues suggest how well someone’s brain can adapt and find workarounds. So, while Cognitive Reserve is real and supported by neuroscience, it’s more of a useful concept than a precise number.
.
Spaced Repetition
Instead of cramming, you review information at intervals over time. This keeps it fresh and easy to recall, with less effort (Cepeda et al., 2006). Active recall, testing yourself instead of just re-reading also boosts long-term memory and reduces mental strain.
.
Thinking Patterns
Your thinking patterns matter too. Psychologist Daniel Kahneman (2011) explains that our brains run on two systems. System 1 is fast and automatic. System 2 is slower and logical. Good habits move helpful tasks from System 2 to System 1, making them effortless and freeing up energy for bigger decisions.
How do you do this? By using habit science in your environment to make new habits stick. This makes good behaviours automatic like brushing your teeth, so they don’t drain your mental energy.
.
Metacognition
Another powerful tool. It means thinking about thinking (Flavell, 1979). When you check in on how you’re learning, spot mistakes, and adjust, you train your brain to be flexible. This flexibility is a core part of Cognitive Reserve.
.
Mindfulness
Simple meditation can strengthen focus and working memory while lowering stress, a major threat to brain health (Zeidan et al., 2010). Just a few minutes a day can keep your mind clear and balanced.
The Mildot course, The Brain Game, combines some of these proven methods. We teach you how to blend spaced repetition, active recall, and metacognition. You’ll learn to shift tasks to your brain’s automatic system, lower cognitive load, and build lasting habits that protect your mind.
In short, Cognitive Reserve isn’t just about luck or genes. It’s about doing small, smart things every day. When you know how to train your memory and shape your thinking habits, you build a brain that can handle life’s surprises and keep you performing at your best.
.
Article References
Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L. and Park, D.C., 2018. Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral Aging. Oxford University Press.
Cepeda, N.J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J.T. and Rohrer, D., 2006. Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), pp.354–380.
Flavell, J.H., 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), pp.906–911.
Gollwitzer, P.M., 1999. Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), pp.493–503.
Kahneman, D., 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. Penguin Books.
Stern, Y., 2009. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), pp.2015–2028.
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S.K., Diamond, B.J., David, Z. and Goolkasian, P., 2010. Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), pp.597–605.